Exploring longitudinal pathways from intelligence to morbidity and mortality risk
View/ Open
Calvin2012.doc (12.12Mb)
Date
28/11/2012Author
Calvin, Catherine Mary
Metadata
Abstract
Human population-based studies of longitudinal design observe that higher
intelligence in youth confers protection from premature mortality in adulthood. This
field of study (“cognitive epidemiology”; Deary & Batty, 2007) has firmly
established associations between intelligence and health outcomes, and has begun to
address the likely mechanisms involved. The present thesis assessed some social,
educational, and lifestyle factors that potentially confound and/or mediate the
intelligence-mortality link.
First, I carried out a systematic review of longitudinal cohort studies
reporting intelligence differences in youth in relation to adult mortality risk, and in
meta-analysis I aggregated the effect sizes from 16. A one SD advantage in
intelligence scores was associated with 24% (95% CI 23% to 25%) lower risk of
death, during 17- to 69-year follow-up; this magnitude showed no sex differential.
Socioeconomic status in early life did not explain the effect. Rather, the person’s
own occupational status in adulthood and educational attainment explained a third
and a half of the association, respectively.
One issue in controlling for education, in such models, is its strong
correlation with intelligence test performance, which could lead to statistical
overadjustment. A second aspect of this thesis, therefore, addressed the nature of the
intelligence-education covariance in two behaviour-genetic studies of large general
population-based samples of schoolchildren from England and The Netherlands.
Previous studies that reported intelligence—education genetic covariances were
potentially biased in their use of twin self-selection or pre-selection sampling.
Moreover, the analysis in this thesis used a novel statistical approach, and included
non-twin data to represent fully the variance in performance scores of a population.
Analysis of the English cohort confirmed the top end of estimates from previous
studies: 76% to 88% of the phenotypic correlation was due to heritability. The Dutch
cohort showed greater variance for equivalent estimates (33% to 100%). The results
indicate a limit to the extent to which education and intelligence might be causative
of one another suggesting caution in interpreting some of the substantive attenuation
effects by education reported in the literature. Third, I investigated pathways from intelligence to cardiovascular disease
risk factors, given the consistent and robust finding that an advantage in intelligence
relates to lower cardiovascular disease-outcomes. I used data from the 1958 National
Child Development Study to investigate age-11 intelligence in association with
inflammatory and haemostatic biomarker status at age 46 years. The results
replicated inverse associations previously reported in an older age sample, and a one
SD advantage in intelligence related to a 1.1mg/L decrease in C-reactive protein. The
effect was largely mediated by lifestyle factors, including smoking, occupational
status and abdominal obesity. In two further studies I used the west of Scotland
Twenty-07 cohort, to investigate processing speeds among 16, 36 and 56 year-olds in
relation to: (1) Inflammation, and (2) metabolic-risk, after 20 years. The advantage of
experimental rather than psychometric measures of cognitive ability is their reduced
cultural and social bias. Faster reaction time predicted lower systemic inflammation
in the youngest male cohort, which appeared to be partially confounded by baseline
smoking and socioeconomic status. Furthermore, advantage in reaction time
performance in the young and middle-aged cohorts significantly predicted reduced
metabolic risk. This was partially explained by occupational status, but retained
statistical significance in some fully-adjusted models. A one SD advantage in age 16
simple reaction time variability, related to the 21% (95% CI 12% to 30%) reduced
odds of metabolic syndrome by age 36 in the basic model, and this effect remained
unchanged after controlling for all covariates.
The growing evidence for specific social and behavioural factors that mediate
intelligence-to-mortality pathways are discussed, in respect of indirect evidence that
underlying system integrity or early life confounding may contribute incrementally
to the effect.